Stress Three Views of Stress 1. 2. 3. Focus on the environment: stress as a stimulus (stressors) Reaction to stress: stress as a response (distress) Relationship between person.

Download Report

Transcript Stress Three Views of Stress 1. 2. 3. Focus on the environment: stress as a stimulus (stressors) Reaction to stress: stress as a response (distress) Relationship between person.

Stress
Three Views of Stress
1.
2.
3.
Focus on the environment: stress as a
stimulus (stressors)
Reaction to stress: stress as a
response (distress)
Relationship between person and the
environment: stress as an interaction
(coping)
Stressors

Some examples?
Stressors






War
Overcrowding
Deadlines
Dense traffic
Marital conflict
Work stress
Acute vs. Chronic Stress

Acute stress


Sudden, typically short-lived, threatening
event (e.g., robbery, giving a speech)
Chronic stress

Ongoing environmental demand (e.g.,
marital conflict, work stress, personality)
Acute Stress
Acute Stress – Rozanski 1988


Subjects – 39 individuals with coronary
artery disease
Stress tasks (0-5 minutes each):




Mental arithmetic
Stroop-colour word conflict task
Stress speech (talk about personal fault)
Graded exercise on bicycle (until chest pain
or exhaustion)
Acute Stress – Rozanski 1988

Outcome – stress response

Myocardial ischemia determined by
radionuclide ventriculography (measures
wall motion abnormalities in the heart)
Acute Stress – Rozanski 1988
Results


Cardiac wall motion abnormalities were
significantly greater with stress speech
than other mental stress tasks (p < .05)
and was of the same order of magnitude
as that with graded exercise.
Wall motion abnormalities occurred with
lower heart rate during stress than during
exercise (64 vs. 94 beats/min, p < .001)
Chronic Stress –
Frankenhauser, 1989



Subjects – 30 managerial and 30
clerical workers
Equal number of men and women
Outcome: blood pressure, heart rate,
and catecholamines measured
throughout workday and non-workday.
Chronic Stress –
Frankenhauser, 1989


No gender differences in the effect of
work on BP and HR.
In both men and women, BP and HR
were higher on a workday than a nonworkday.
Chronic Stress –
Frankenhauser, 1989
Catecholamine Response
3
2.5
2
Women
Men
1.5
1
0.5
0
10:00
12:00
14:00
16:00
Time of Day
18:00
20:00
Three Views of Stress
1.
2.
3.
Focus on the environment: stress as a
stimulus (stressors)
Reaction to stress: stress as a
response (distress)
Relationship between person and the
environment: stress as an interaction
(coping)
Fight or Flight Response





Increase in
Epinephrine &
norepinephrine
Cortisol
Heart rate & blood
pressure
Levels & mobilization of
free fatty acids,
cholesterol &
triglycerides
Platelet adhesiveness &
aggregation

Decrease in
Blood flow to the
kidneys, skin and gut
Selye’s General Adaptation
Syndrome (1956, 1976, 1985)
Perceived
Stressor
Alarm
Reaction
•Fight or
flight
Resistance
•Arousal high
as body tries
defend and
adapt.
If stress continues ….
Exhaustion
•Limited
physical
resources;
resistance
to disease
collapses;
death
Cognitive Model of Stress
Lazarus & Folkman




Potential stressor (external event)
Primary appraisal – is this event
positive, neutral or negative; and if
negative, how bad?
Secondary appraisal – do I have
resources or skills to handle event?
If No, then distress.
Cognitive Model of Stress
Lazarus & Folkman



Primary appraisal – Is there a potential
threat?
Outcome – Is it irrelevant, good, or
stressful?
If stressful, evaluate further:



Harm-loss – amount of damage already
caused.
Threat – expectation for future harm.
Challenge – opportunity to achieve growth, etc
Cognitive Model of Stress
Lazarus & Folkman

Secondary appraisal –

Do I have the resources to deal effectively with
this challenge or stressor?
Cognitive Model of Stress
Lazarus & Folkman
High Threat Low
Resources
High Stress
High Threat High
Resources
High
Demands
High/low
demands
Low Threat Low
Resources
Low
demands
Some
stress
Low Threat High
Resources
Low
demands
Low or no
stress
Moderate
Stress
Personal Factors Affecting
Stress Appraisal




Intellectual
Motivational
Personality
Beliefs
Situational Factors Affecting
Stress Appraisals







Strong demands
Imminent
Life transition
Timing
Ambiguity – role or harm ambiguity
Desirability
Controllability


Behavioural control – perform an action
Cognitive control – using a mental strategy
Learned Helplessness –
Seligman, Peterson, et al.



Dogs exposed to unavoidable shocks
Following exposure, when placed in a
situation where they can now jump to
avoid the shock, they fail to make the
escape response.
Learned helplessness occurs when one
perceives that one’s actions (e.g.,
working hard) does not lead to the
expected outcome (e.g., high grade).
Job Strain – Karasek et al.,
1981
Demands
High
High
Control
Low
STRAIN
Low
Job Stress – other aspects






Physical environment
Poor interpersonal relationships
Perceived inadequate recognition or
advancement
Unemployment (even anticipated)
Role conflict
High responsibility for others
Biopsychosocial Aspect of
Stress

How stress affects health


Via behaviour
Via physiology
Behavioural Aspects





Increased alcohol
Smoking
Increased caffeine
Poor diet
Inattention leading to carelessness
Physiological Aspects



Cardiovascular reactivity – increased
blood pressure, platelets, lipids
(cholesterol)
Endocrine reactivity – increased
catecholamines and corticosteroids
Immune reactivity – increased
hormones impairs immune function
Psychophysiological Disorders



Digestive system – e.g., ulcers, irritable
bowel syndrome
Respiratory system – e.g., asthma
Cardiovascular system – e.g.,
hypertension, lipid disorders, heart
attack, angina
Stress-Illness Relationship
Illness
Preexisting
physiological
or psychological
vulnerability
Physiological
& psychological
wear and tear
Exposure
to stress
Behavioural
changes &
Coping efforts
Illness
precursors,
symptoms
Illness
behaviour
Moderators of the Stress
Experience
What is coping?



Process of managing the discrepancy
between the demands of the situation
and the available resources.
Ongoing process of appraisal and
reappraisal (not static)
Can alter the stress problem OR
regulate the emotional response.
Emotion-Focused Coping




Aimed at controlling the emotional response to the
stressor.
Behavioural (use of drugs, alcohol, social
support, distraction) and cognitive (change the
meaning of the stress).
Often used when the person feels he/she can’t
change the stressor (e.g., bereavement); or
Doesn’t have resources to deal with the demand.
Problem-Focused Coping


Aimed at reducing the demands of the
situation or expanding the resources for
dealing with it.
Often used when the person believes
that the demand is changeable.
Coping responses – respond
yes or no.
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
7.
Tried to see the positive side of it.
Tried to step back from the situation and be
more objective.
Prayed for guidance or strength.
Sometimes took it out on others when I felt
angry and depressed.
Got busy with other things to keep my mind off
the problem.
Read relevant material for solutions and
considered several alternatives.
Took some action to improve the situation.
Problem-Focused Coping


Planful Problem-Solving – analyzing the
situation to arrive at solutions and then
taking direct action to correct the
problem.
Confrontive Coping – taking assertive
action, often involving anger or risk
taking to change the situation.
Emotion-Focused Coping






Seeking social support – can be either problem or
emotion-focused coping.
Distancing – cognitive effort to detach
Escape-avoidance – wishful thinking or taking
action to escape or avoid it.
Self-control – attempting to modulate one’s
feelings in response to the stressor.
Accepting responsibility – acknowledging one’s
role in the situation while trying to put things
right.
Positive reappraisal – create positive meaning.
Cognitive Re-structuring

Process by which stress-provoking
thoughts are replaced with more
constructive one.
Gender and Coping



Men generally employ problem-focused coping
strategies more than emotional focused strategies.
Opposite for women, with women more often
employing emotion-focused strategies.
If men and women in same occupation, gender
differences disappear, suggesting that societal sex
roles influence choice of coping strategies.
Socio-economic Status (SES)
and Coping


People with higher SES tend to use
problem-focused coping strategies more
often (Billings & Moos, 1981).
Why do people who have lower SES use
problem-focus coping strategies less often
than those with high SES?
Personality or Coping Style



Negative affectivity
Pessimism – optimism
Hardiness
Life Orientation Test
(Scheier & Carver)
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
In uncertain times, I usually expect the best.
If something can go wrong for me it will.
I always look on the bright side.
I’m always optimistic about my future.
I hardly ever expect things to go my way.
Things never work out the way I want them to.
I’m a believer in the idea that “every cloud has
a silver lining.”
I rarely count on good things happening to me.
Overall, I expect more good things to happen
to me than bad.
Personality or Coping Style



Negative affectivity
Pessimism – optimism
Hardiness
Social Support





Emotional support – expression of empathy,
understanding, caring, etc.
Esteem support – positive regard,
encouragement, validating self-worth
Tangible or instrumental – lending a helpful
hand.
Information support – providing information,
new insights, advice.
Network support – feeling of belonging
Factors Influencing Utilization or
Availability of Social Support


Temperament – people differ in their needs
for social support. Social support can be
detrimental if you are the type of person
who likes to handle things on your own.
Previous experience with social support
influences your likelihood of seeking out
social support in the future.
Threats to Social Support




Stressful events can interfere with your
ability to use social supports.
People under stress may become so focused
on talking about their problems that they
drive their support systems away.
Supports agents may react in a way that
makes the problem worse.
Support providers may be adversely
effected by providing support.
Alxheimer’s Disease (AD) – Effect on
Caregivers




Subsample of the Cardiovascular (CVD)
Health Study, a prospective study of risk
factors for CVD in the elderly.
Excluded: disabled confined to wheel chair,
unable to attend field centres, or undergoing
cancer treatment.
Caregivers defined as those whose spouse
had difficulty with one activity of daily living
due to physical or mental health problem.
392 caregivers and 427 non-caregivers
recruited.
AD – Effect on Caregivers




Caregivers were asked to rate the degree of
mental and physical strain associated with
caregiving (3-point response format).
Sample subdivided into four groups: noncaregivers; spouse disabled but not helping
him/her; caregiver but no reports of strain; and
caregiver with reports of strain.
Followed for 4.5 years (range 3.4 – 5.5 years).
Main outcome – mortality (100% follow-up
achieved).
AD – Effect on Caregivers
Results



81% of caregivers were providing care.
56% reported caregiver strain.
Mortality – 9.4% in non-caregivers;
17.3% in ‘caregivers’ not providing care;
13.8% in non-strained caregivers; and
17.3% in strained caregivers.
Generally Social Support
Associated with Good Effects



Increase survival rates in women who have
breast cancer.
Lower blood pressure
Decrease risk of mortality
Psychological Predictors of
Sudden Cardiac Death in
CAMIAT
J. Irvine, A. Basinski, B. Baker, S.
Jandciu, M. Pickett, J. Cairns, S.
Connolly, M. Gent, R. Roberts, &
P. Dorian,
Psychos Med 1999
Funded by Heart and Stroke Foundation of
Ontario
Psychosocial Predictors of Sudden Cardiac
Death in CAMIAT
Measures:




Cook-Medley Index: measures of hostility,
anger, cynicism
Beck Depression Inventory
Symptom Checklist-90: psychological distress
Social Support: measures of social participation,
network and perceived social support
Psychosocial Predictors of Sudden
Cardiac Death
Variable
Relative
Risk
2.86
1.37 – 5.99 0.005
Hx CHF
3.86
1.89 – 7.89 0.001
Depress. – P
2.48
1.14 – 5.35 0.02
Depress. - A
0.52
0.15 – 1.76 0.29
Network Cont.
1.04
1.00 – 1.06 0.01
Social Activities
0.98
0.96 – 1.00 0.05
Previous MI
95% CI
p
Stress Management
Stress Management – teaches
coping techniques





Reduce harmful environmental conditions
Teaches techniques by which person can
develop stress tolerance.
Helps client maintain a positive self-image.
Help maintain emotional equilibrium.
Help client maintain or develop satisfying
relations with others.
Cognitive Therapy – Albert
Ellis, Aaron Beck

Assumes that stress arises or is augmented
by faulty or irrational ways of thinking.



Catastrophizing – “It is awful if I get turned
down when I ask for a date”.
Overgeneralizing – “I didn’t get a good grade on
this test. I can’t get anything right”.
Selective abstraction – Only seeing specific
details of the situation (e.g., Seeing the
negatives but missing the positive details).
Cognitive Therapy

Often these irrational beliefs or faulty
thinking errors stem from past
“programming”.


E.g., Not receiving adequate love and
nurturance as a child may lead to feelings that
loved ones in the present don’t “quite love you
enough”.
Hypothesis testing – client is encouraged to
test out these irrational beliefs by collecting
evidence for or against the belief.
Cognitive Therapy


Errors in Information Processing Irrational Thinking Errors include:





Emotional reasoning
Overgeneralization
Catastrophic thinking
Mind reading
Selective negative focus, etc.
Relaxation Therapy

Aims to either reduce hyperarousal or
curb emotional-physiological reactivity.




Progressive muscular relaxation
Mental imagery
Meditation
Autogenic training
Time Management




Set short-term (e.g., daily) and long-term
(e.g., yearly) goals.
Make daily to-do lists (prioritize each).
Make a daily schedule for when and where
you will carry out your to-do list items
(estimate time allocated for each to-do
item).
Revise throughout the day as needed.