下載/瀏覽

Download Report

Transcript 下載/瀏覽

Processing English Compounds in the
First and Second Language:
The Influence of the Middle Morpheme
Author: Victoria A. Murphy & Jennifer Hayes
Presenter: Shu-ling Hung (Sherry)
Advisor: Raung-fu Chung
Date: May 09, 2013
1
Term (1)
• A compound is made up of two or more words
concatenated to form another word.
a “head”
word
a modifier
Compounds
• pan and cake → pancake
• taxi driver
2
Term (2)
3
Term (2-1)
Derivation is the morphological process which
creates a word with a new meaning and/or
category.
4
Term (2-2)
Inflectional morphology is the changes that
happen in words to denote certain grammatical
features.
5
Introduction
• Native English speakers tend to exclude regular plural inflection
when producing English noun-noun compounds (e.g., rat-eater not
rats-eater) while allowing irregular plural inflection within compounds
(e.g., mice-eater) (Gordon, 1985; Lardiere, 1995; Murphy, 2000).
• Exposure to the input is insufficient to explain this dissociation
between regular and irregular plurals in compounds because
occurring compounds in English rarely have plurals of any type
included within them.
• The constraint on the production of plural could be derived from the
patterns in which regular plural and possessive morphemes occur in
the input.
6
Literature Review
Haskell et al. (2003) showed that the influence of the semantic and the
phonetic constraints working in tandem leads to very few plurals that end in
/s/ or /z/ appearing before a noun.
Word +/s/
/z/
plural but
does not
end in /s/
or /z/
N.
N.
rare (the semantic
and the phonetic
constraints)
Maybe ok (the
semantic is invoked)
7
Literature Review
Haskell et al (2003) : constraint satisfaction model
rat catcher
mice catcher
scissors
8
Literature Review
• Cunnings and Clahsen (2007) took issue with Haskell et al.’s (2003)
explanation of why regular plurals are excluded from English
compounds and argued that an important comparison to support Haskell
et al.’s (2003) model would come from compounds with nonhead nouns
that are semantically and morphologically singular but yet nonetheless
sound plural.
• They supported the notion that participants exclude regular plurals from
compounds due to a morphological constraint.
9
Purpose of the Study
• The study was to explore the extent to which these more
input-based or probabilistic explanations of how plural
inflectional morphology and compounding interact might
account for L2 learner behavior.
• How nonhead nouns ending in the phoneme /s/ (or /z/)
are treated in compounds was also tested in this study.
10
Research Questions
1. Will compounds containing possessive nouns be processed more
quickly than compounds containing plural nouns?
2. Will the same preferences as shown by native speakers (NSs) be
manifest by nonnative speakers (NNSs) who have had considerably
less exposure to the input?
3. Will compounds in which the first noun ends in /s/ (/z/), whether it is
the plural form or not, be processed more slowly than compounds
that do not include a first noun ending in /s/ (/z/)? Will this difference
be manifested by the NNSs who have had significantly less
exposure to English?
11
Methodology-- Participants
Native speakers (NSs)
Nonnative speakers (NNSs)
Sex
Male: 1; Female: 21
Male: 5; Female: 8
Mean age
24
23
Education
the Department of Psychology
at the University of
Hertfordshire
UK university for academic study
(advance-level learners of English)
Degree of L2
No
No, but classroom-based foreign
language instruction in China
12
Methodology-- Materials and Stimuli
• The frequencies of these first nouns were calculated
using the analysis in Francis and Kucera (1982).
13
Methodology-- Materials and Stimuli
• The apostrophe was omitted.
• Each compound was preceded by contextualizing sentence, which a
pilot study with NSs had confirmed would lead participants to the
intended interpretation of the first noun in the compound.
• A dummy compound was also tested made up of two nonwords by
changing the letters of the target compounds to yield a nonce
compound item that was phonologically plausible in English.
• Sentences and compounds appeared centered on the computer
screen in 48-point type.
• Psyscope software was used to analyze the study.
14
Methodology-- Appendix
15
Methodology-- Procedure
test individually in an cubicle
categorize a series of compounds
At the beginning of each trial, a contextualizing
sentence appeared on screen. (216 test trials)
press the space bar causing an asterisk to
appear on screen and the sentence to
disappear→read aloud
press the space bar and the compound
appeared
16
Results-- Accuracy Data
• The participants’ responses to the noun-noun
compounds were coded as correct or incorrect in terms
of their acceptability of the legitimate compounds in
English.
• An initial repeated measures analysis of variance was
carried out with Order as the independent-samples factor.
• A repeated measures multivariate ANOVA was carried
out with one independent-samples factor (Group) tested
at two levels (NS, NNS) and one related-samples factor
(Word Type) tested at five levels.
17
Results-- Accuracy Data
The difference in errors for nonhead nouns ending in phoneme /s/ (/z/) and possessive
[-s] was reliable.
Neither group had any difficulty correctly distinguishing the real words in the
compounds from the nonce compounds.
over 90%
accurate
Over 80%
accurate
18
Results-- Reaction Time Data
• An initial ANOVA with Order as the only independentsamples variable was carried out to determine whether
Order had an influence on how quickly participants
responded on the LDT.
• An overall (omnibus) F test was carried out with one
independent-samples factor (NS, NNS) and one relatedsamples factor(Word Type).
19
Results-- Reaction Time Data
For all different types of nonhead nouns, the Chinese NNSs were slower
to respond on the LDT than the English NS.
20
Results-- Comparison
Comparisons was carried out on the NS and NNS RT data to determine the
extent to which differences in responding were found across the relevant
types of nonhead nouns.
quickly
more
longer
quickly
higher than
possessive
& slower
Harder to
process
→less
input
Haskell et al.’s idea but support the
suggestion
21
Summary
• Native speakers of English processed compounds with medial
possessive morphology faster than compounds with medial regular
plural morphology.
• The second language learners did not show the same pattern as the
NSs, which could be due to the fact that they had considerably less
exposure to the relevant input patterns relative to the NSs.
• Regular plurals may be excluded before a rightmost noun in English
because the pattern “Noun–[-s] morpheme–Noun” is more
frequently used for marking possession in English.
• Irregular plurals do not end in the [-s] morpheme and do not
“compete” with the possessive marker. Consequently, they may be
optionally included in compounds.
22
Limitations of the Study
• small sample sizes →larger sample sizes
• unequal variability across groups → even
number of male and female subjects
• limitations of the stimuli → different types of
processing tasks and stimuli
23
Comments
There are some factors that can be investigated deeply
in the topic.
• How much input is required before a learner of English
comes to work out the relative patterns of where different
aspects of morphology appear in English grammar?
• How long does it take for a learner to learn the sequence
“noun–[-s]–noun” more frequently marks possession?
• It would be profitable to use more sophisticated
measures, such as eye movements.
24
25