Overlooking stimulus variance
Download
Report
Transcript Overlooking stimulus variance
Overlooking Stimulus Variance
Jake Westfall
University of Colorado Boulder
Charles M. Judd
University of Colorado Boulder
David A. Kenny
University of Connecticut
Cornfield & Tukey (1956):
“The two spans of the bridge of inference”
My actual
samples
50 University of Colorado
undergraduates;
40 positively/negatively
valenced English adjectives
Ultimate targets
of generalization
My actual
samples
All healthy, Western adults;
All non-neutral visual stimuli
50 University of Colorado
undergraduates;
40 positively/negatively
valenced English adjectives
Ultimate targets
of generalization
All
potentially sampled
participants/stimuli
My actual
samples
All healthy, Western adults;
All non-neutral visual stimuli
All CU undergraduates taking
Psych 101 in Spring 2014;
All short, common, strongly
valenced English adjectives
50 University of Colorado
undergraduates;
40 positively/negatively
valenced English adjectives
Ultimate targets
of generalization
All
potentially sampled
participants/stimuli
My actual
samples
All healthy, Western adults;
All non-neutral visual stimuli
“Subject-matter span”
“Statistical span”
50 University of Colorado
undergraduates;
40 positively/negatively
valenced English adjectives
Difficulties crossing the statistical span
• Failure to account for uncertainty associated with
stimulus sampling (i.e., treating stimuli as fixed
rather than random) leads to biased, overconfident
estimates of effects
• The pervasive failure to model stimulus as a random
factor is probably responsible for many failures to
replicate when future studies use different stimulus
samples
Doing the correct analysis is easy!
• Modern statistical procedures solve the statistical
problem of stimulus sampling
• These linear mixed models with crossed random
effects are easy to apply and are already widely
available in major statistical packages
– R, SAS, SPSS, Stata, etc.
Illustrative Design
• Participants crossed with Stimuli
– Each Participant responds to each Stimulus
• Stimuli nested under Condition
– Each Stimulus always in either Condition A or Condition B
• Participants crossed with Condition
– Participants make responses under both Conditions
Sample of hypothetical dataset:
5
4
6
7
3
8
8
7
9
5
6
5
4
4
7
8
4
6
9
6
7
4
5
6
5
3
6
7
4
5
7
5
8
3
4
5
Typical repeated measures analyses (RM-ANOVA)
5
4
6
7
3
8
8
7
9
5
6
5
4
4
7
8
4
6
9
6
7
4
5
6
5
3
6
7
4
5
7
5
8
3
4
5
How variable are the stimulus ratings
around each of the participant means?
The variance is lost due to the aggregation
“By-participant analysis”
MBlack
MWhite
Difference
5.5
6.67
1.17
5.5
6.17
0.67
5.0
5.33
0.33
Typical repeated measures analyses (RM-ANOVA)
5
4
6
7
3
8
8
7
9
5
6
5
4
4
7
8
4
6
9
6
7
4
5
6
5
3
6
7
4
5
7
5
8
3
4
5
4.00 3.67 6.33 7.33 3.67 6.33 8.00 6.00 8.00 4.00 5.00 5.33
Sample 1
v.s.
Sample 2
“By-stimulus analysis”
Simulation of type 1 error rates for
typical RM-ANOVA analyses
• Design is the same as previously discussed
• Draw random samples of participants and stimuli
– Variance components = 4, Error variance = 16
• Number of participants = 10, 30, 50, 70, 90
• Number of stimuli = 10, 30, 50, 70, 90
• Conducted both by-participant and by-stimulus
analysis on each simulated dataset
• True Condition effect = 0
Type 1 error rate simulation results
• The exact simulated error rates depend on the
variance components, which although realistic,
were ultimately arbitrary
• The main points to take away here are:
1. The standard analyses will virtually always show
some degree of positive bias
2. In some (entirely realistic) cases, this bias can be
extreme
3. The degree of bias depends in a predictable way on
the design of the experiment (e.g., the sample sizes)
The old solution: Quasi-F statistics
• Although quasi-Fs successfully address the
statistical problem, they suffer from a variety of
limitations
– Require complete orthogonal design (balanced factors)
– No missing data
– No continuous covariates
– A different quasi-F must be derived (often laboriously)
for each new experimental design
– Not widely implemented in major statistical packages
The new solution: Mixed models
• Known variously as:
– Mixed-effects models, multilevel models, random
effects models, hierarchical linear models, etc.
• Most psychologists familiar with mixed models
for hierarchical random factors
– E.g., students nested in classrooms
• Less well known is that mixed models can also
easily accommodate designs with crossed
random factors
– E.g., participants crossed with stimuli
Grand mean = 100
MeanA = -5
MeanB = 5
Participant
Means
5.86
7.09
-1.09
-4.53
Stimulus Means: -2.84
-9.19
-1.16
18.17
Participant
Slopes
3.02
-9.09
3.15
-1.38
Everything else = residual error
The linear mixed-effects model
with crossed random effects
Fixed effects
Random effects
Fitting mixed models is easy: Sample syntax
R
SAS
SPSS
library(lme4)
model <- lmer(y ~ c + (1 | j) + (c | i))
proc mixed covtest;
class i j;
model y=c/solution;
random intercept c/sub=i type=un;
random intercept/sub=j;
run;
MIXED y WITH c
/FIXED=c
/PRINT=SOLUTION TESTCOV
/RANDOM=INTERCEPT c | SUBJECT(i) COVTYPE(UN)
/RANDOM=INTERCEPT | SUBJECT(j).
Mixed models successfully maintain
the nominal type 1 error rate (α = .05)
Conclusion
• Stimulus variation is a generalizability issue
• The conclusions we draw in the Discussion sections
of our papers ought to be in line with the
assumptions of the statistical methods we use
• Mixed models with crossed random effects allow us
to generalize across both participants and stimuli
The end
Further reading:
Judd, C. M., Westfall, J., & Kenny, D. A. (2012).
Treating stimuli as a random factor in social
psychology: A new and comprehensive solution to a
pervasive but largely ignored problem. Journal of
personality and social psychology, 103(1), 54-69.