Meta-analysis - University of Auckland

Download Report

Transcript Meta-analysis - University of Auckland

Meta-analysis: pooling study
results
Simon Thornley
Objective
• Understand the philosophy of meta-analysis and its
contribution to epidemiology and science.
• Understand the limitations of meta-analysis
Introduction
• Systematic quantitative integration of results several
independent studies
• Distinct from a narrative review “expert”
• Synthesis of published information.
• Usually considered only appropriate for RCTs
• Still controversial even in this context.
• Google search on “meta-analysis” 8 million hits!
Criticism
• “statistical alchemy” for the 21st Century
• “The intellectual allure of making mathematical models
and aggregating collections of studies has been used
as an escape from the more fundamental scientific
challenges”
•
-Feinstein.
Purposes of meta analysis
• Inefficiency of traditional narrative reviews.
• Allow researchers to keep abreast of accumulating
evidence
• Resolution of uncertainty when research disagrees?
• Increase statistical power, enhances precision of effect
estimates – especially small effects
• Allows exploratory analysis (subgroups)
Inadequate sample size? (Deal with
type-2 error)
• Single trials too small to detect moderate effects
• (low power – high chance of Type-2 error (falsenegative))
• Investigators often over enthusiastic about size of treatment
effects and sample size
• Meta-analysis doesn’t deal with other threats to study
validity (bias, measurement error; in fact, may increase)
• e.g. CVD death vs. total mortality
Statistical Test result
Accept H0
True
OK
Reject H0
Type-1 error
H0
False
Type-2 error
OK
Prob of a type 1 error = alpha a (usually fixed, say 0.05)
Prob of a type 2 error = beta b= 1-power
Random error lecture
Average odds ratio is 21?? Consistency??
Which studies?
• Need defined question, state MESH terms
• Reproducible
• Exhaustive search
• Unpublished and published studies
• Variety of databases.
Typical summary outcome measures
Binary:
•
•
•
•
•
Relative risk
Odds ratio
Risk difference
NNT [=1/RD]
Incidence rate ratios
(person time data)
Continuous:
• Difference in means,
• Standardized
differences in means
• Survival measures
Methods of analysis
Fixed effect
Random effect
•
•
Assume distribution of true effects
•
Aim is to measure mean of distribution
of true effects
•
Greater heterogeneity --> greater
variation
•
Gives greater weight to small studies
than fixed effect method of analysis.
•
More conservative (wider confidence
interval around effect estimate,
compared to fixed effect method)
Mantel-Haenszel method
•
•
O-E (Peto) method
•
Binary outcome (e.g. death)
•
Oi =observed # deaths on treatment in
trial i
•
Ei=expected # deaths (assuming no
treat effect)
•
•
treat each trial as a “stratum” take
weighted average of effects.
look at average of Oi - Ei over all trials
Assumes underlying true effect for each study
and differences only due to random error
Dietary fat and cholesterol
Reduced or modified dietary fat and allcause mortality
Publication bias
When meta-analysis goes bad…
• In CVD drug research, CVD outcomes
often favoured over total mortality
• Which would you prefer????
Publication bias: other methods
• Ioannidis JPA, Trikalinos TA. An exploratory test for an
excess of significant findings. Clin. Trials 2007;4(3):245-53.
• Calculate expected number of positive studies, given:
• Sample size of individual studies
• Number of events in controls
• Summary effect (assumed true)
Statin meta-analysis
Problems
• Combining heterogeneous studies (apples and oranges)
• Combining good and bad studies (good and bad apples)
(study quality)
• Publication bias
(tasty apples only)
• The "Flat Earth" criticism – reductionism –(Braeburns only)
• Combining data (individual v summary data stewed
apples have different character to raw)
• Application to randomized studies only?
• Type-2 error only one problem with epi studies
Meta analysis in observational studies
• MA often applied in observational studies
• As often as RCTs (Egger et al)
• …. with controversy ….
• Confounding and bias unlikely to “cancel out”
• Publication bias and “research initiation bias” (i.e. studies only
done when there is an association)
• Different ways of reporting/analysing result (e.g different
outcome measures, confounders, models, exposure levels)
Summary
• Meta-analyses increasingly used
• Logical only for RCTs?
• Summarise medical literature
• Reduce type-2 error by increasing sample size.
• Don’t deal with other types of epidemiological error
(confounding/measurement error)
• Prone to unique type of error (Publication bias)
• Can be difficult to detect